Jessica M. Szczuka * and Nicole C. Krämer
Social Psychology: Media and Communication, University Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany;
nicole.kraemer@uni-due.de
* Correspondence: jessica.szczuka@uni-due.de; Tel.: +49-203-379-1293
Academic Editors: Adrian David Cheok, Cristina Portalés Ricart and Chamari Edirisinghe
Received: 30 September 2016; Accepted: 6 February 2017; Published: 10 February 2017
Abstract: While some theories, such as the Media Equation, suggest that men will evaluate sex robots
to be attractive, other assumptions (e.g., biases of norm adherence) would contradict this hypothesis.
Therefore, the present study aimed at empirically comparing men’s explicit and implicit evaluation
of the (sexual) attractiveness of sex robots and women. At the same time, the personal characteristics
of the observer that might affect this evaluation were considered. An online survey (n = 229) and
an effective priming experiment (n = 41) revealed that men rate women to be more attractive than
robots if asked explicitly (=self-reported). However, this effect is not present when attractiveness is
assessed implicitly (unbiased, directly). Moreover, affiliation-related traits such as loneliness, which
have been assumed to be associated with the usage of sex dolls, are not related to the evaluation of
attractiveness. Instead, a negative attitude towards robots is an important predictor.
Keywords: sex robot; explicit and implicit measure; personality traits; attractiveness; HRI
1. Introduction
Although sex robots represent an emerging field of application for humanoid robots, research
on the sexual aspects of human-robot interaction is almost non-existent. With the rise of humanoid
robots, more and more companies are focusing on building android robots (robotic replications of
humans). As the adult industry is known for driving technological developments, such as Internet
applications or virtual reality [1], it can be expected that the use of robots for the satisfaction of sexual
needs will soon be rendered possible due to the technological progress in the field of sex robots.
Sex dolls already have a hyper-realistic outward appearance [2]. Companies such as True Companion
or Abyss Creations are already working on different robotic solutions to enable the human replications
to move and talk. David Levy, one of the first researchers to discuss love and sex with robots in detail,
predicted that by the year 2050 robots will be capable of being “perceived as being similar to biological
creatures” [3] (p. 303) and that this will make robots appealing in terms of sexual and affectional
purposes. While the media coverage often associates the usage of sex dolls or sex robots with being
lonely (e.g., “Lonely men to get guide on building a sex robot” [4]), a first study by Schuetz and Arnold,
with a relatively small male sample size, revealed that more than two-thirds of the male participants
could imagine using a sex robot [5]. This demonstrates that, as futuristic as it might seem, the topic
might be of interest to more than merely a fringe group. Indeed, theoretical considerations based on
social psychological insights into the mechanisms of physical attractiveness as well as assumptions
from communication science, such as the Media Equation (the process of applying social rules to
computers [6]), would suggest that attraction to sex robots could be a universal phenomenon. However,
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3; doi:10.3390/mti1010003 www.mdpi.com/journal/mti
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 2 of 18
other concepts, such as the uncanny valley (a negative reaction if robots are too human-like [7]) or
sexual and social norms would predict that sex robots would not be appealing to the masses [2].
While research in different fields of human–robot interaction is on the rise (e.g., human–robot
interaction in healthcare), empirical research on the sexual aspects of human–robot interaction is almost
completely lacking. Although there are some scientific papers on sex robots, they mostly discuss
ethical concerns (e.g., [8,9]). While such considerations are indeed highly valuable, as the development
of sex robots might be related to various negative societal outcomes (e.g., objectification of people),
empirical studies focusing on the user’s perception and evaluation are nevertheless necessary to gain a
deeper understanding of sexual aspects in human–robot interaction.
In this vein, the present study aimed not only to investigate how men evaluate sex robots if
asked explicitly (=self-reported) and the influence of personality traits on this explicit evaluation,
but also whether the same results emerge if an immediate, and therefore unbiased, reaction is used
to measure the attractiveness perception (affective priming). This multi-methodological perspective
enables a more profound understanding of the evaluation of sex robots. Moreover, it allows for an
empirical investigation into the importance of different aspects, such as bias of sexual and societal
norm adherence, the evolutionary principles of attractiveness, or the influence of certain personal
characteristics. It is important to gain in-depth knowledge about the influence of personality traits, as
research in the context of human–robot interaction has already shown that the way humans think about
and act towards robots is influenced by personal characteristics and previous experiences. Taking
into account personal characteristics is particularly crucial with regard to sex robots as there is a
stereotypical claim that lonely people, who have a hard time getting in touch with real people, could
be particularly attracted to sex robots in order to fulfill their sexual and affectional needs [4].
The study is separated into two parts: Study 1a (n = 229) comprises an online questionnaire
assessing men’s explicit evaluations of women and robots and the personal characteristics that might
have an influence on the rating. In Study 1b, 41 additional participants took part in an affective priming
paradigm to access the subliminal attractiveness ratings of women and robots. This was important as
we aimed to investigate the immediate reaction to sex robots. To enable a comparison of the results,
the two studies used the same stimulus material.
1.1. Sex Robots
Sex robots can be understood as android robots (robotic replications of the appearances of women
or men) that are built to satisfy sexual needs. For this purpose, the robots provide not only replications
of some secondary sexual characteristics (e.g., breasts), but also external genitals (e.g., labia). As this
description could also apply to hyper-realistic sex dolls, sex robots are also able to move (which is
particularly important with regard to the fulfillment of sexual needs) and speak. Affective computing
will be an important aspect of sex robots since having sex is one of the most intimate interactions
possible, in which reacting and acting like a human will be perceived as quality attributes. At present,
there is no sex robot available that is suitable for the masses and that provides all of the features
listed above. However, there are first approaches, such as the sex robot Roxxxy by the company True
Companion or the first moving body parts created by the company Abyss Creations. David Levy
predicts that, by 2050, technological achievements will make it possible to fall in love with, have
sex with, and even get married to a robot [3]. On the other hand, as mentioned above, these new
developments have given way to ethical concerns. Among other aspects, the possible process of
objectification (especially with respect to prostitutes) needs to be kept in mind, as well as the illusion of
love (caused by the illusion of interaction with a person), which can lead to various ramifications [8,9].
1.2. Sex and Attraction
Sexual intercourse between humans is one of the most intimate interactions people
can have [10,11]. Although the concept of sex is often associated with long-lasting romantic
relationships, sexual experiences can manifest themselves in different ways. Studies have demonstrated
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 3 of 18
that, for men, there are different positive psychological and physiological effects of sexual experiences,
caused, for instance, by masturbation (e.g., [12]) or by sexual intercourse in short-term relationships
(e.g., [13]). Despite the lack of consensus regarding whether sex drive is based on psychological or
physiological processes or an integrated process, every human experience sexual tension [14–16].
In this respect, research has shown that men have a higher need for sex compared to women, who
tend to have a stronger need for affiliation [17].
The precondition for sexual activities (especially with regard to intercourse) is an attraction. Research
on evolutionary principles of mate selection shows that, in particular, waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) plays
an important role in this regard [18,19]. The WHR represents the body fat distribution between the
upper and lower body, which is influenced by sex hormones that cause the visual difference between
women and men after puberty. Singh further argues that “WHR appears to be the most accurate signal
of female reproductive capability” [19] (p. 315), which is the main factor in mate selection. Besides the
WHR, breast size also plays an important role in the evaluation of attractiveness. In an eye-tracking
study, Dixson et al. found that men’s initial visual fixation is either on the breasts or the waist, not on
the face or lower body—at least when a naked woman is shown [20]. In a similar study conducted by
Hewig et al., in which participants were presented with pictures of dressed women and men, the male
participants’ initial look was at the face of women, followed by a fixation on the breasts [21]. The face is
another important influence on attractiveness ratings, as it transmits a variety of information including
sociodemographic information such as gender or age, but also information on emotions and physical
health [22–24]. Familiarity has been shown to be another important aspect for attractiveness ratings, as
we react positively to known things [25].
Ratings on how people evaluate an individual or a category of people (e.g., men or women) to be
(physically) attractive are usually measured by explicit ratings, such as self-reporting questionnaires.
However, there are first studies using subliminal measurements to assess the strength of the relationship
between the concept of attractiveness and categories of stimulus material. This could be of special
interest in cases in which attractiveness ratings might be influenced by biases of norm adherence, which
might lead to socially desired behavior. For instance, Snowden et al. used subliminal measurements to
show that sexual orientation and erotic preference can be captured using implicit measurements [26].
1.3. Media Equation
Reeves et al. demonstrated in various studies that people behave the same way in interactions
with computers as they would with humans (e.g., they are polite, they apply gender stereotypes)
(e.g., [6,27]). The effect of this so-called Media Equation (media equals real life) has also already been
shown with regard to robots (e.g., experiencing empathy, keeping interpersonal distance) [28,29].
In terms of intimate interactions with robots, Li et al. conducted a study in which the humanoid
robot NAO told the participants to either touch or point to different body parts (with low and high
accessibility) [30]. The results showed that people experienced increased physiological arousal
and increased reaction time when touching regions of the robot’s body with lower accessibility
(e.g., buttocks or genitals). The authors discussed this finding in the context of the Media Equation.
As the study did not focus on the question of why the arousal increased, the authors assumed (among
other reasons) that touching low-accessible body parts of a human-like robot might evoke a feeling of
discomfort, based on social norms or the increased intimacy. Although the authors did not discuss this
in particular, it can be assumed that the increased arousal cannot be compared to sexual arousal, as the
humanoid NAO is 57.3 cm/22.6” tall, made of white plastic, and is sometimes even described as being
cute in a childlike way [30,31]. Nevertheless, their finding is important, as it shows that touching even
remotely human-shaped robots evokes arousal. It is therefore conceivable that androids might evoke
even stronger reactions, in the sense of actual sexual arousal, since anthropomorphism (in design and
behavior) is an important aspect in the Media Equation. Ferrari stated that human appearance in robots
is not only relevant on a functional level (some things can simply be done better with human features,
e.g., raising a glass), but also on a physiological level, as we tend to interpret anthropomorphic
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 4 of 18
features as “human” features [32]. For instance, Bartneck et al. demonstrated that the degree of
anthropomorphism (in their study: moving lips, eyes, and eyebrows) had an influence on feelings of
embarrassment in an interaction with a medical robot [33]. The authors discussed this finding in the
context of Media Equation assumptions. They pointed out that there are certain situations in which
a less human-like appearance could be helpful, but also noted that highly anthropomorphic robots
could have benefits with regard to human-robot interaction. One aspect that could have an influence
on the perceived human likeness is salient mechanical body parts. These come with the consequence
that humans can instantly categorize them as robots, as their human-like outward appearance is
disturbed, for instance, by plastic or metallic components, and this, in turn, could trigger negative
pre-existing attitudes towards robots. However, if robots have no visual robotic components at all, it is
possible that they might evoke feelings of eeriness, as described in the context of the uncanny valley
phenomenon [7].
1.4. Uncanny Valley
The theory of the uncanny valley states that robots evoke more positive feelings of acceptance the
more human-like they become until their evaluation is influenced by the so-called uncanny valley.
The current generation of android robots often triggers this effect, as they are built to copy human
beings but fail to achieve complete humanness due to restrictions in appearance and/or behavior.
This misguided representation of humans is accompanied by negative evaluations/feelings [7].
The model predicts that the human likeness of a robot and its acceptance by the user are not related in
a linear manner. One frequently suggested explanation is the perception-oriented approach, which
suggests that very human-looking robots often do not meet people’s expectations (e.g., with regard to
haptic aspects or social norms). The resulting uncertainty (in the sense of difficulty in categorizing the
entity) leads to negative evaluations [34,35].
Referring to the original uncanny valley model, robots without any salient mechanical body
parts should be located somewhere on the upper-right of the curve after the dip, representing nearly
perfect humanness [7]. In contrast, robots with visual cues (e.g., buttons or wire) allow an instant
classification and therefore reduce uncertainties due to their salient mechanical body parts. Therefore,
they should be located somewhere on the left of the curve, before the uncanniness starts. Theoretically,
both positions in the model could lead to an average likeability [7]. However, Bartneck et al. found
that there was no difference between the likeability of a human and his android doppelgänger, which
demonstrates that the uncanny valley is not always empirically supported [36]. Moreover, it should be
emphasized that research on the phenomenon is strongly influenced by the measures and stimuli used.
In particular, the movement can have an important influence on results with regard to the uncanny
valley hypothesis [28].
1.5. Social and Sexual Norms
Concerning aspects that might influence the evaluation of sex robots, sexual norms could be of
particular importance. Nowadays, technology plays an important role in sexuality [37,38]. The term
technosexuality describes sexual activities that are combined with technology. Some technosexual
behaviors, such as Internet pornography, are more common than others. So far, sexual activities
involving robots have been described as deviant and termed robot fetishism, being defined as a “fetish
attraction to humanoid or non-humanoid robots, or to people behaving like robots, or to people
dressed in robot costumes” [37] (p. 66/67). Although possible users might be unaware of the fact
that such sexual behavior could be labeled as fetishism, it is clear that sexual activities with an object
deviate from statistical sexual norms [2], which in turn could be evaluated negatively. Moreover, these
sexual norms could lead to difficulties in the context of empirical research, as participants have the
tendency to respond in ways they believe to be socially acceptable (social desirability).
Another aspect that could lead to negative attitudes towards sex robots is that having sex with a
doll, instead of a human, is associated with being lonely or desperate [37]. Although a robot is more
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 5 of 18
lifelike than a doll, it is still a machine, and it can, therefore, be assumed that this stereotype could
transfer to sex robots, as both are non-human. However, it should be stressed that sex dolls (ranging
from cheap inflatable versions to expensive ones made of silicone) and also the technology used for sexual
stimulation is already part of the present sex toy industry [37,39].
1.6. Influence of Personal Characteristics on Human-Robot Interaction
As already briefly mentioned in the context of the uncanny valley, the perception of robots can be
influenced by different personality traits and pre-existing biases towards robots. In the following,
personality traits and their influence on robots and the evaluation of attractiveness will be discussed.
1.6.1. Affiliation-Related Variables in the Context of (Sex) Robots
When thinking about a sex doll or future sex robot owners, the stereotype prevails (often portrayed
by the media, e.g., in the news such as an article in the Times with the headline “Lonely men to get
guide on building a sex robot” or in the film Lars and the Real Girl) that people who are attracted
by artificial entities suffer from loneliness or are unable to get in touch with other people [4,40].
David Levy also addressed this topic in his book Love and Sex with Robots, stating in his conclusion that
“Many who would otherwise have become social misfits, social outcasts, or even worse will instead be
better-balanced human beings.” [3] (p. 304). In the documentary Love Me, Love My Doll/Guys and Dolls
produced by Holt, who looked at owners of life-sized sex dolls called “Real Dolls”, one 32-year-old
man confirmed Levy’s conclusion, stating that: “I can tolerate being alone, but not loneliness” [3,41].
Although there are no empirical results regarding sex robots, research has shown that lonely people
do benefit from contact with humanoid robots. For instance, Eyssel and Reich demonstrated that
lonely people tend to anthropomorphize humanoid robots more strongly (with respect to their
characteristics) [42]. This is a highly relevant finding because people have a fundamental motivation
to affiliate with others [43], which in turn might be satisfied by interactions with humanoid robots.
Moreover, it is conceivable that people who have problems satisfying this need to belong, such as
people with social anxieties, might especially benefit from interactions with humanoid robots. Such
interactions might provide a feeling of safety with respect to aspects or behaviors that could be
controlled in a robot, such as fear of rejection [44]. This aspect might be of special importance for sex
robots, as people suffering from social anxieties often have problems engaging in interactions with
the opposite sex and consequently have fewer sexual experiences in their lives [45]. A first empirical
hint that people suffering from social anxieties can benefit from interactions with robots was provided
by Suzuki et al., who examined persons scoring highly on social avoidance and distress [46]. Such
individuals indicated that they would prefer robots over humans as communication partners in various
situations (e.g., asking for directions at a station or on a street).
1.6.2. Pre-Existing Attitudes
Another aspect that needs to be kept in mind with regard to attractiveness in robots is that
robots, as a concept, are associated with certain pre-existing positive and negative biases [47].
In this context, Nomura et al. investigated the anxieties people have towards robots with regard
to situations of interaction with robots, the social influence of robots, and emotions robots might
have [48]. They developed Negative Attitudes towards Robots Scale (NARS), which is an important
instrument in research on human-robot interaction. Different studies showed that the attitudes
measured by the NARS have an influence on various evaluations in human-robot interactions.
For instance, the attitudes have an influence on the distance people keep between robots and themselves
and the willingness to engage in physical contact with robots [49]. As important as this latter aspect
is, it has to be noted that this state of anxiety (evoked in certain situations) is strongly linked to a
robot’s appearance (e.g., human-sized or small humanoids) [48]. In this regard, no research has been
conducted so far that focuses on sex robots and pre-existing attitudes.
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 6 of 18
1.7. Hypotheses
The aim of the present study is not only to examine whether men evaluate female android robots
to be attractive, especially in comparison to how they rate women, but also whether this evaluation is
influenced by certain personality traits and biases of norm adherence or social desirability. Based on
the presented literature review, the following hypotheses were derived:
There are different theoretical approaches (especially the Media Equation and evolution-based
mechanisms of attractiveness) that would suggest that men evaluate female androids (with or without
salient mechanical body parts) to be as attractive as women. However, there are also visual differences
between women and robots, especially with respect to robots with salient mechanical body parts that
might disturb the impression of human likeness. Moreover, the influences of social norms, as well
as the possibility that female android robots with and without salient mechanical body parts might
give rise to feelings of uncanniness, would speak against this hypothesis. Research on the evaluation
of attractiveness showed that erotic preferences can be measured using subliminal measurements.
This might be particularly relevant for attractiveness ratings that could be biased by norm adherence
and social desirability [26]. Since no study has yet empirically investigated differences in attractiveness
ratings among different forms of robots and between robots and women, the following research
question is posed:
Research Question 1 (RQ1). Is there a difference in men´s explicit evaluation of the (sexual) attractiveness of
women, female androids with salient mechanical body parts, and female androids without salient body parts?
To date, no research has been conducted on the influence of biases of norm adherence on the
evaluation of sex robots. However, it seems likely that the evaluation of attractiveness, which is usually
an aspect that would only be rated with regard to humans, is influenced by aspects that need time for
reflection (such as social or sexual norms). In the present study, a subliminal measurement (effective
priming) is used in order to access the implicit, that is, subliminal evaluation of the attractiveness
of women and robots. This could have the consequence that the participants are unable to retrieve
social norms or social desirability. In turn, this would mean that participants rely on the first visual
impressions they get. Although the robots used in the affective priming paradigm do have salient
mechanical body parts that allow an instant categorization, it can be assumed that the evolution-based
mechanisms of attractiveness trigger the same reactions as would pictures of women. Therefore, the
following hypothesis is formulated:
Hypothesis 1 (H1). The affective priming paradigm (implicit measurement) will show no difference between
women and robots in the associative strength with regard to the concept of attractiveness.
Sex robots are built to fulfill sexual needs. Among humans, this need is most often fulfilled by
physical contact with other human beings and is therefore connected to affiliation-related personal
characteristics, such as fear of rejection or loneliness. Therefore, we aim to investigate the influence
of affiliation-related personality traits on the evaluation of sex robots. Moreover, it is important to
empirically investigate the stereotypical claim that users of sex robots suffer from loneliness or are
unable to get in touch with other people [4,40].
In particular, men suffering from loneliness, fear of rejection, or interaction deficits and men who
rate social contacts to be unimportant might benefit from interactions with female robots, as they
might, for instance, be able to control their fears in human-robot interaction. The anthropomorphic
the tendency was already shown to be important for lonely people in human-robot interaction, as lonely
people tend to anthropomorphize humanoid robots more strongly [42], and research in the field of
human-robot interaction showed that a negative attitude towards robots has an influence on how
people behave towards them (e.g., with regard to the willingness to engage in physical contact [49]).
In line with this and the literature review in Section 1.6, two hypotheses are derived, which
explicitly (H2) and implicitly (H3) test the importance of the mentioned personality traits:
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 7 of 18
Hypothesis 2 (H2). Loneliness, the importance of social contacts, fear of rejection, the individual degree of
interaction deficits, anthropomorphic tendency, and a negative attitude towards robots are predictors of the
evaluation of the attractiveness of robots with and without salient mechanical body parts.
As already mentioned in the context of Hypothesis 1, implicit measurements allow direct,
unbiased insight into the associative strength between concepts. Based on this method, we wanted
to empirically test whether affiliation-related personal characteristics can serve as predictors of the
direct, unbiased reaction regarding the attractiveness of robots. In other words, it is conceivable that
(for instance) loneliness and fear of rejection predict how strongly the concept of attractiveness is
connected to sex robots, meaning how quickly the participants react in the affective priming paradigm.
Therefore, Hypothesis 3 extends Hypothesis 2 by using a subliminal measurement (like in H1) to
examine the influence of affiliation-related personality traits on the evaluation of attractiveness
of robots.
Hypothesis 3 (H3). Loneliness, the importance of social contacts, fear of rejection, the individual degree of
interaction deficits, anthropomorphic tendency, and a negative attitude towards robots are all predictors of the
associative strength of the concept of attractiveness and robots (measured by reaction time between the pictures
of robots and a positive/attractive word).
To test the research question and the hypotheses, this study is separated into two parts; Study 1a
is an online survey (n = 229) that aims to investigate the explicit evaluations (=self-reported) of
attractiveness with regard to women and sex robots (with and without salient mechanical body parts)
(RQ1) and the influences of personality traits on this evaluation (H2). Study 1b, on the other hand, is
an experiment that uses the affective priming paradigm to gain unbiased insights into the associative
strength of attractiveness of women compared to the associative strength of attractiveness of sex robots
(H1), and again, the influence of personal characteristics on this implicit evaluation (H3). This method
forces an immediate, direct reaction of the participants without any bias of norm adherence or influence
of social desirability. Although both samples filled in the same questionnaire, to ensure that the data of
Study 1a was not biased by the additional affective priming paradigm completed by the participants
of Study 1b, the data were kept separate.
2. Materials and Methods for Study 1a (Explicit Measure of Perceived Attractiveness of Robots
and Women)
2.1. Sample and Procedure
To investigate the explicit evaluations of sex robots, a total of 229 heterosexual male participants
aged from 18 to 67 (M = 25.82, SD = 6.85) took part in an online survey; 53.3% (n = 122) of the
participants indicated that they were in a relationship, while 107 (46.7%) were single.
The present study focused on male participants only, which is based on three important aspects:
(1) Attitudes relating to sexual activities differ strongly between men and women (e.g., [17]); (2) an
an empirical study conducted by Schuetz and Arnold showed that men were significantly more in favor
of sex robots, or the idea of using one, compared to women; and (3) the product sector focuses more
on the male consumer by producing mainly female sex dolls or (first) sex robots [5].
The survey was composed of three parts. First, the participants were asked to watch a two-minute
video showing female robots, such as Sophia (by Hanson Robotics) and HRP-4C (Miim; by the National
Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST)). We aimed to create an understanding
of what state-of-the-art robots look like (e.g., facial expressions, secondary sexual characteristics), what
their abilities are (e.g., standing and walking), and in which fields of application they can be used
(e.g., domestic worker or training object for dentists). The second part comprised different personality
measures, which will be explained in more detail in the next section. In the third part, the participants
rated the (sexual) attractiveness of four women in underwear, four female robots in underwear with
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 8 of 18
salient mechanical body parts, and four female androids (biologically correct replication of women)
in underwear, presented in pictures. There was a note under every picture clarifying whether the
picture displayed a woman or a robot in order to ensure that the participants would understand the
different concepts, especially with respect to the robots without any salient mechanical body parts.
To prevent the three kinds of pictures from influencing each other, we pre-structured them such that the
participants first had to rate a picture of a woman, followed by a picture of a female robot with salient
mechanical body parts, and then a picture displaying a female robot without salient mechanical body
parts. This process was repeated four times with different pictures in order to minimize the influence
based on the appearance of a single picture and in order to reduce possible effects of randomization
(e.g., cognition on one category if, for instance, three pictures of women were shown in a row instead
of providing a new category in each picture in order to foster a focus on the perception of the picture
and not the category).
All participants were able to look at the pictures for as long as they wished. They were then asked
(with regard to the robot pictures) whether they would buy such a robot for themselves now or within
the next five years and to indicate why. In an open question, they were asked what they evaluated to
be attractive and unattractive with regard to the female robots in underwear with salient mechanical
body parts and the female androids (biologically correct replication of women) in underwear. Finally,
each participant was debriefed and had the chance to win one of three gift certificates ( 50).
In total, 238 participants were recruited, but nine had to be excluded from the analysis as the
corresponding box plot identified these participants as outliers.
2.2. Stimulus Material
An example of the pictures of the women is shown in Figure 1 (left). Pictures of sex dolls were
used to represent the robots without any mechanical body parts (Figure 1, middle) and the pictures
showing the robots with salient mechanical body parts were edited by merging heads of sex dolls with
computer-generated bodies that have obvious visual distortions of the skin (salient mechanical body
parts; Figure 1, right).
Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2017, 1, 3 8 of 18
picture clarifying whether the picture displayed a woman or a robot in order to ensure that the
participants would understand the different concepts, especially with respect to the robots without
any salient mechanical body parts. To prevent the three kinds of pictures from influencing each other,
we pre-structured them such that the participants first had to rate a picture of a woman, followed by
a picture of a female robot with salient mechanical body parts, and then a picture displaying a female
a robot without salient mechanical body parts. This process was repeated four times with different
pictures in order to minimize the influence based on the appearance of a single picture and in order
to reduce possible effects of randomization (e.g., cognition on one category if, for instance, three
pictures of women were shown in a row instead of providing a new category in each picture in order
to foster the focus on the perception of the picture and not the category).
All participants were able to look at the pictures for as long as they wished. They were then
asked (with regard to the robot pictures) whether they would buy such a robot for themselves now
or within the next five years and to indicate why. In an open question, they were asked what they
evaluated to be attractive and unattractive with regard to the female robots in underwear with salient
mechanical body parts and the female androids (biologically correct replication of women) in
underwear. Finally, each participant was debriefed and had the chance to win one of three gift
certificates (€50).
In total, 238 participants were recruited, but nine had to be excluded from the analysis as the
corresponding box plot identified these participants as outliers.
2.2. Stimulus Material
An example of the pictures of the women is shown in Figure 1 (left). Pictures of sex dolls were
used to represent the robots without any mechanical body parts (Figure 1, middle) and the pictures
showing the robots with salient mechanical body parts were edited by merging heads of sex dolls
with computer-generated bodies that have obvious visual distortions of the skin (salient mechanical
body parts; Figure 1, right).
Figure 1. Examples of the stimulus material of the women (left); the robots without salient mechanical
body parts (middle); and the robots with salient mechanical body parts (right). Please note that these
three categories were used in Study 1a. In Study 1b (affective priming), only pictures of women (left)
and the robots with salient mechanical body parts (right) were shown.
Several aspects were taken into account to ensure comparability of the pictures of the female
robots with salient mechanical body parts, the female androids, and the pictures of the women: All
of the robots and women wore black or white basic underwear (underpants and bra or something
similar to cover secondary sex characteristics), they were all shown against a white background, all
of the pictures showed the same image section, and they all had a neutral to sexy expression on their
faces (to control for likeability, for instance, none of them had an extreme smile). All 12 pictures
shown were selected based on a pretest (n = 10). In this pretest, seven pictures of women, six pictures
Figure 1. Examples of the stimulus material of the women (left); the robots without salient mechanical
body parts (middle); and the robots with salient mechanical body parts (right). Please note that these
three categories were used in Study 1a. In Study 1b (affective priming), only pictures of women (left)
and the robots with salient mechanical body parts (right) were shown.
Several aspects were taken into account to ensure comparability of the pictures of the female
robots with salient mechanical body parts, the female androids, and the pictures of the women: All of
the robots and women wore black or white basic underwear (underpants and bra or something similar
to cover secondary sex characteristics), they were all shown against a white background, all of them
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 9 of 18
pictures showed the same image section, and they all had a neutral to sexy expression on their faces
(to control for likeability, for instance, none of them had an extreme smile). All 12 pictures shown were
selected based on a pretest (n = 10). In this pretest, seven pictures of women, six pictures of robots
with salient mechanical body parts and six pictures of robots without salient mechanical body parts
were evaluated regarding attractiveness and likeability on a five-point Likert scale. Moreover, the
participants had to indicate on a five-point Likert scale whether the shown robots were realistic or
unrealistic to them. Based on this pretest, the pictures (four in each category) were selected that had a
similar mean regarding firstly, attractiveness and secondly, likeability. In addition, the means of the
robots’ perceived realism were computed to exclude pictures of robots that look highly unrealistic.
Figure 1 shows examples of the final stimulus material.
2.3. Measures
In the following, all relevant variables are explained. Please note that all measures had to be
answered on a five-point Likert scale (1 = “disagree strongly” to 5 = “agree strongly”). Moreover, two
additional measures (a self-developed scale measuring the concept of suspension of disbelief and a
self-developed scale measuring the importance of the social aspects of sex) could not be used in further
analyses due to an unsatisfactory internal consistency ( 0.5).
2.3.1. (Sexual) Attractiveness
Each participant evaluated the pictures of the female robots and women by means of seven
semantic differentials (e.g., “sexually unattractive vs. sexually attractive”). Although the pairs of
words were pretested to ensure that they covered the concept of (sexual) attractiveness, 12 exploratory
factor analyses with principal component analysis and varimax rotation were computed, followed
by Horn’s parallel analyses [50]. For every calculation of each picture of the robots and women, one
factor was suggested based on the eigenvalue criteria. Instead of only computing one-factor analysis,
we decided to run one for all pictures to explore possible differences between the items if robots
or women were rated. The 12 exploratory factor analyses with principal axis analysis and Promax
rotation revealed that overall, the items “appealing”, “sexually attractive”, “beautiful”, “exciting”,
and “likable” had the highest factor loadings regardless of whether women or robots were shown.
Therefore, these items were summed up to measure attractiveness with an overall internal consistency
of = 0.954.
2.3.2. Anthropomorphic Tendency
To assess whether the participants have the “tendency to ascribe human characteristics to
non-human objects” (p. 214), the Anthropomorphism Questionnaire by Neave et al. was used [51].
The scale was originally developed to measure the influence of anthropomorphic tendencies on
hoarding. However, the 20 items, such as “As a child, I sometimes said ‘hello’ and ‘good night’ to
some of my favorite toys” are worded neutrally and could, therefore, be used in the present study.
The internal consistency was = 0.877.
2.3.3. Negative Attitudes towards Robots
The NARS scale by Syrdal et al. is based on work by Nomura et al. and covers negative attitudes
towards social/future implications, emotional attitudes and action interactions [9,48]. The 11 items
(e.g., “I would feel uneasy if robots really had emotions”) had an internal consistency (Cronbach’s
alpha) of = 0.824. As the internal consistency of the subscales was unsatisfactory ( 0.5), the
the overall score was used.
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 10 of 18
2.3.4. Loneliness
The revised UCLA Loneliness scale by Russel et al. was used to assess participants’ subjective
feelings of loneliness [52]. The 20 items (e.g., “People are around me but not with me”) had an internal
consistency of = 0.912.
2.3.5. Need to Belong (Importance of Social Contacts)
The subscale “importance of social contacts” of the Need to Belong Scale by Krämer et al. was
used to measure the importance of contact with others in everyday life [53]. The subscale consists
of five items (e.g., “I frequently think of my loved ones”). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha)
was = 0.772.
2.3.6. Social Anxiety (Fear of Rejection and Interaction Deficit)
The SASKO Scale of Social Anxiety by Sabine Kolbeck contains the subscales “fear of rejection”
and “interaction deficit” [44]. Each subscale contains five items, such as “I am afraid of situations in
which I could get rejected by somebody of the opposite gender.” and “I feel uneasy at parties because
I don’t know how to behave”. The subscale “fear of rejection” had a Cronbach’s alpha of = 0.816,
while the subscale “interaction deficit” reached an internal consistency of = 0.756.
3. Results of Study 1a
3.1. Differences in Attractiveness between Robots and Women (RQ1)
To investigate whether there is a difference in men’s explicit evaluation of the (sexual)
the attractiveness of women compared to female robots with salient mechanical body parts and female
androids (biologically correct replication of women), a one-way repeated measures ANOVA with a
Greenhouse–Geisser correction was computed. It should be noted that the ratings for each group
(robots with salient mechanical body parts, female androids, and all women) were summed up.
The results showed a significant difference between the ratings of (sexual) attractiveness between the
female types (F(1.94, 442.95) = 370.30, p 0.01). The Bonferroni post hoc test revealed that all of them
pairwise comparisons differed significantly from another. The results showed that women (M = 3.82,
SD = 0.54) had the highest attractiveness score, followed by female androids (biologically correct
replication of women) (M = 2.80, SD = 0.81) (p = 0.01), and female robots with salient mechanical body
parts (M = 2.66, SD = 0.74) (p 0.01).
3.2. Influence of Personality Traits for the Explicit Measurement (H2)
To test whether negative attitude towards robots, perceived loneliness, the importance of social
contacts, fear of rejection, interaction deficits, or anthropomorphic tendencies would significantly
predict the ratings of attractiveness of robots with and without salient mechanical body parts, two linear
regression analyses were computed.
The results of the linear regression for the attractiveness of robots without salient mechanical
body parts showed that the regression model explained 14.4% of the variance (F(6222) = 7.38, p < 0.01),
with the negative attitude towards robots (Beta = 0.40, t(222) = 6.34, p < 0.01) and fear of rejection
(Beta = 0.18, t(263) = 2.54, p = 0.01) as the only two significant predictors.
The linear regression for the attractiveness of robots with salient mechanical body parts showed
that 23.5% of the variance could be explained by the model (F(6222) = 12.70, p < 0.01). However, a
more detailed look at the coefficients revealed that the negative attitude towards robots was the only
significant predictor (Beta = 0.48, t(222) = 8.03, p < 0.01).
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 11 of 18
4. Materials and Methods for Study 1b (Implicit Measure of Attraction towards Robots and
Women (Affective Priming))
The second study aimed to investigate the implicit evaluation of sex robots. The main idea
of the affective priming paradigm is that the participants have to react as quickly as possible
to words representing attractiveness presented for 500 ms. This enabled us to investigate the
immediate reaction to sex robots, without any biases of norm adherence. Using this effective priming
paradigm [54], the associative strength between women and attractiveness/unattractiveness and
robots and attractiveness/unattractiveness was assessed with the help of reaction times. The basic
idea is that participants are able to assign a word (positive or negative) more quickly to its category
(positive or negative) if the prime (shown picture) and the target category are congruent (have a
stronger associative strength) compared to incongruent trials.
Twenty-four pictures of women in underwear and 24 pictures of robots in underwear with salient
mechanical body parts were used as primes. It should be noted that the displayed robots all had
salient mechanical body parts to ensure that the participants would recognize the robots, as they
were only shown for 500 ms. Moreover, 24 words connected to the concept of (sexual) attractiveness
(positive; e.g., “affectionate”, “beautiful”, “gentle”) and 24 words associated with the concept of (sexual)
unattractiveness (negative; e.g., “unappealing”, “rejection”, “unfeeling”) represented the targets.
Each participant performed 48 practice trials and 48 prime-target test trials. Within the practice
trials, the participants had to assign each target word, which was presented in the middle of the screen,
to the category attractive or unattractive. The categorization was realized by clicking on the “E” key of
the keyboard for the attractive category or “I” for the unattractive category. For instance, if the word
“beautiful” appeared, the participants had to click on the “E” key as quickly as possible. The category
names “positive/attractive” and “negative/unattractive” were permanently displayed in the right
and left corner of the screen and did not change between the trials. Within the actual test trial, the
primes, pictures of women and robots, were displayed for 500 ms, followed by 100 ms black screen
(Interstimulus Interval (ISI)), before the target word appeared. Affective priming paradigms often have
a Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA) of less than 500 ms in order to ensure that participants are not able
to respond intentionally [54]. However, we consciously chose a slightly longer SOA of 600 ms to ensure
that the participants were able to perceive the visual difference between the displaced women and the
female robots (recognizable by the obvious mechanical body parts). The participants were instructed
to focus on the assignment of the words, but also to look at the pictures. Both the practice and the test
trials contained all target words (positive and negative) in a random order (without any duplicates).
4.1. Procedure
The experiment was separated into three phases. After entering the room, all participants first
watched the same video (2 min) as the participants in Study 1a, displaying state-of-art female robots,
in order to achieve an understanding of what robots look like, what their abilities are and in which
fields of application they can be used. Following this, the examiner entered the room again in
order to explain the affective priming paradigm. If no further question arose, the participants started
the affective priming task. All participants received additional standardized instructions from the
computer. It is important to mention that the examiner highlighted that the pictures used in the
paradigm displayed women and robots. After the effective priming task, the participants took part in
Study 1a. (The analyses of Study 1a do not contain these 41 participants.)
4.2. Stimulus Material
The 24 pictures of the robots and the 24 pictures of the women were composed of 12 different
pictures each, which were simply mirrored because, especially with regard to the robot pictures, there
were not as many pictures that could be easily aligned/retouched. All of the pictures were pretested to
ensure that they would be realistic and to exclude outliers with regard to attractiveness. Some of the
pictures were used in Study 1a as stimulus material for the explicit attractiveness ratings. Examples
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 12 of 18
are shown in Figure 1. The words used in the affective priming paradigm were also pretested to ensure
that they could be assigned to one category (positive/negative) without any ambiguities.
4.3. Sample
The analyses were based on the data of 41 heterosexual male participants aged between 18 and
52 (M = 25.41, SD = 6.43) who participated in the affective priming task. Twenty-four participants
(58.5%) indicated that they were single, while 17 (41.5%) were in a relationship. The reaction times
varied between 555.79 ms and 1372.33 ms (M = 791.03, SD = 180.61). An a priori power analysis was
conducted with a power coefficient of 0.80 and an effect size of 0.40, which was also found in a study
by Avero and Calvo, who used a paradigm similar to that in the present study [55]. The analysis
showed that 40 participants would represent good sample size.
In total, 46 participants took part in the affective priming paradigm, but two participants had to
be excluded due to misstatements given by the examiner concerning the affective priming paradigm,
and three participants had to be excluded based on too long average reaction times (more than 1500 ms
on average, which was probably caused by performance problems of the computer).
The participants received a payment of 5 and were recruited via social media sites and the
local newspaper.
5. Results of Study 1b
Overall, the mean latencies were 792.00 ms (SD = 191.30) for the incongruent pairs and 790.06 ms
(SD = 187.35) for the congruent pairs, which leads to a priming effect of 1.94 ms. A paired t-test on the
latencies showed no significant differences (t(40) = 0.10, p = 0.92).
5.1. The difference in Reaction Times between Pictures of Women and Robots with Regard to Attractive/Positive
Words (H1)
To test whether the attractive/positive target words could be assigned faster if a picture of a
woman was shown compared to a picture of a female robot, which would represent the associative
strength between each of those concepts, again, a paired t-test was computed. The descriptive data
show that there was almost no difference in the time taken to assign the word to the attractive category
when comparing the presentation of robot pictures (M= 766.21, SD = 188.41) and the pictures of women
(M = 770.98, SD = 254.17). Accordingly, no significant difference was found (t(40) = 0.14, p = 0.89).
Therefore, Hypothesis 1 was confirmed. The concept of attractiveness was not more accessible when
seeing pictures of women compared to seeing pictures of robots.
5.2. Personality Traits as Predictors (H3)
To test whether the negative attitude towards robots, the perceived loneliness, the importance of
social contacts, fear of rejection, interaction deficits, or anthropomorphic tendencies were significant
predictors of the reaction time with regard to pictures of robots followed by a positive/attractive
word, a linear regression analysis was computed. The results showed that none of the aforementioned
personality traits significantly predicted the reaction time between robots and attractive/positive
words (F(6) = 0.86, p = 0.54). Therefore, Hypothesis 3 needs to be rejected.
6. Discussion
With the rise of companies working on humanoid robots, sex robots will enter the market very
soon. However, the topic is biased by societal stereotypes and norm adherence. The main purpose of the
present study was to investigate not only whether men perceive women to be more sexually attractive
compared to robots, but also whether this evaluation would differ if the men were asked explicitly and
implicitly. Moreover, we aimed to gain more insights into possible personal characteristics that would
influence this evaluation. We found that the explicit rating differed from the implicit rating insofar as
the differences in the attractiveness ratings between women and robots were not present when the
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 13 of 18
men were asked implicitly. Moreover, we found that personal characteristics related to social life had
no influence on the sexual attractiveness ratings.
6.1. Explicit Rating of Attractiveness with Regard to Women and Robots (RQ1)
The present study showed that when asked explicitly, men evaluate women to be more attractive
than robots without salient mechanical body parts and robots with salient mechanical body parts.
This finding should be discussed in the context of two aspects: familiarity and norm adherence. Firstly,
the grading is important, as it shows that women, representing the most familiar stimulus category,
were evaluated as more attractive than both kinds of robots. This is in line with the argument that
humans react more positively towards things or people with whom they are familiar because we
cannot tell whether unknown objects or people are associated with danger [25,26]. Such an argument
is underlined by the fact that it is very likely that the participants had not previously interacted with
sex robots, leading them to rate the women as more attractive. This can also be transferred to both
kinds of robots tested, as those without salient mechanical body parts were evaluated to be more
attractive than those with salient mechanical body parts. The mechanical body parts, represented by
wires or obvious plastic or metallic material components, not only distract the visual impression of
human likeness but might also play an important role with regard to sexual interactions people may
have with them, as physical contact could be associated with a risk of injury or unpleasantness of
touch. However, it should be mentioned that the descriptive statistics of the (sexual) attractiveness
ratings of the robots indicated that the mean values were medium rather than low, possibly due to the
fact that the (mechanical) robots also show different physical aspects that are known to be important
in attractiveness evaluations, such as a low WHR or large breast size [19,20]. The other aspect that
needs to be highlighted is that the participants were aware that they were rating robots (especially
in comparison with women) and that this, in turn, may have influenced the evaluations. Sexual
interactions with non-humans are seen as sexually deviant, and users of sex robots or sex dolls are
frequently associated by the media with being desperate and lonely [37]. As they had sufficient time
to think about the concept of robots in general and society’s evaluation of them in particular, it can be
assumed that participants reflected not only on social norms but also on the social desirability of their
answers within the empirical setting.
In general, it has to be noted that 40.3% of the present sample indicated that they could imagine
buying a sex robot now or within the next five years. This is fully in line with the results of Schuetz
and Arnold, who showed that more than two-thirds of their male participants could imagine using a
sex robot [5]. However, this willingness of so many participants to indicate such an interest speaks
against the importance of social desirability.
Overall, as we did not investigate the motivations for the ratings, more research is required on
stereotypes of sex robot users and the effect of salient mechanical body parts for robots in the field of
applications with physical contact between human and robots.
6.2. Influence of Personal Characteristics on Explicit Attractiveness Evaluation (H2)
With regard to the influence of personal characteristics on the attractiveness ratings, the results
showed that the negative attitude towards robots was the only significant predictor for both the robots
with and without salient mechanical body parts. Moreover, fear of rejection was a significant predictor
for the robots without salient body parts.
First of all, it should be highlighted that the post hoc test revealed that the negative attitude
towards robots is a strong negative predictor for both robot versions. This is fully in line with research
by Nomura et al., who showed that the negative attitude towards robots has an important impact on
the evaluations of situations in which robots and humans get closer, meaning that it influences the
allowed distance between robots and humans and the willingness to engage in touch with robots [49].
As these are aspects that play a huge role in the context of sexual interactions, the predictor is plausible.
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 14 of 18
The finding of a positive association between the indicated fear of rejection and the attractiveness
rating of the robots without salient mechanical body parts may point to the importance of the control
of sex robots. In contrast to the robots without salient mechanical body parts, they look the same as
women, but in contrast to women, they can be expected to not reject the user. However, it is important
to keep in mind that we did not investigate the underlying reasons for the importance of specific personal
characteristics. Therefore, this is an aspect that needs to be investigated in more detail in future studies.
Nevertheless, both calculations reflect the title of the present study, meaning that the
affiliation-related characteristics of perceived loneliness, the importance of social contacts and
interaction deficits, all failed to significantly predict the attractiveness ratings of the robots with
and without salient mechanical body parts. This result shows that the picture of a lonely user who
is not capable of bonding with real people and instead uses a sex doll or robot was not confirmed
empirically (e.g., [4,37]).
6.3. Implicit Rating of Attractiveness with Regard to Women and Robots (H1)
Another main finding is that the previously discussed finding of differences in the explicit
attractiveness ratings between women and robots was not present in the implicit ratings, meaning
that for the present sample there was no difference in the associative strength between the concept of
attractiveness with regard to women and robots. The main difference between the two methods lies
in the time which the participants had to reflect on their attractiveness ratings. First, this could have
the consequence that the men had to rely on their first visual perceptions of the robots. Dixson et al.
showed that men’s initial visual fixation focuses either on the breasts or the waist and as the shown
robots do provide these features, it can be assumed that our participants—on an immediate and
subliminal basis—reacted in a similar way to how they would react with regard to women [20]. This is
in line with assumptions from evolutionary psychology that men are predisposed to attend and react
to these cues as they are associated with, for instance, female reproductive capability [19]. Therefore,
the result highlights the possible importance of human-like visual cues in sex robots. Second, the
instant reaction forced the participants not to reflect on social or sexual norms, which might have led
to more negative reactions (as discussed in the context of the explicit evaluations (RQ1)).
6.4. Influence of Personal Characteristics on Implicit Attractiveness Evaluation (H3)
The results regarding the influence of personal characteristics (negative attitude towards
robots, perceived loneliness, importance of social contacts, fear of rejection, interaction deficits, or
anthropomorphic tendencies) showed that none of the mentioned traits serves as a significant predictor
of the reaction time with regard to pictures of robots followed by a positive/attractive word. This needs
to be discussed by taking into account the results of the regression of the explicit attractiveness ratings.
Here, the negative attitude towards robots was the only predictor that was significant for both the
attractiveness ratings of the robots with and without salient mechanical body parts. It is possible
that the instant reaction forced the participants to focus on the information provided in the short
duration time (500 ms). As it is possible that the first impressions are simply based on the basic visual
information (waist, breast size; see discussion of H1) and do not take into account the (recognizable)
fact that the female-looking figure is a robot, it is possible that the negative attitude towards robots did
not play such a large role and therefore did not serve as a significant predictor of the reaction times.
On the one hand, this raises the question of whether the participants really perceived the difference
between the shown robots and the women in the context of the implicit measurement. On the other
hand, it highlights the importance of research on human-like visual cues in the context of sex robots.
6.5. Limitations and Future Research
One limitation of the present study is that the affective priming paradigm had a Stimulus Onset
Asynchrony (SOA) of 600 ms, which is 100 ms more than suggested by Fazio et al. [54]. It could
therefore be argued that the stimulus material was presented for too long and that the results could
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 15 of 18
have been based on intentional response. As the visual differences between the pictures of the
women and the robots were mainly based on the salient mechanical body parts, we nevertheless
deemed it more important to ensure that the participants would perceive the difference between the
categories. The non-significant results, which differ from the explicit ratings, indeed support this
decision. A further limitation might be seen in the selection of the participants, as we recruited them
as a convenience sample on social networking sites and newspaper ads, in which we stated that the
study would be about the evaluation of robots and the sexual aspects of human-robot interaction.
We considered this to be important information, as the evaluation included data on intimate topics.
However, it is possible that mainly men who were already interested in the topic of sex robots took
part in the study.
Future empirical studies should consider previous experiences with android robots and
technology that was used to fulfill sexual needs (e.g., vibrator) as possibly important variables. It is
imaginable that those aspects will have an impact on the evaluation of sex robots. Moreover, it could
be interesting to investigate how people would react towards sex robots if they do know for sure
whether they were seeing hyper-realistic sex robots or women. The reaction towards the missing label
could provide more information on the importance of uncertainties in the evaluation of sex robots.
From a more general perspective, we argue that there is a strong need for future research
focusing on the user and his or her evaluation of sex robots. A long-term goal should be to investigate
real interactions between humans and sex robots, as research in the context of the uncanny valley
phenomenon has already demonstrated that results differ based on the form of stimulus material [28].
Since sex robots are mainly built by the adult industry, researchers rely on their technological
developments in order to improve the quality of research. However, first empirical approaches using
different kinds of stimulus material, such as pictures, videos, or virtual reality, and different forms of
qualitative or quantitative measures should be encouraged in order to gain new insights into users’
acceptance of sex robots as well as their effects.
7. Conclusions
The present paper found that, on the one hand, women are rated as more (sexually) attractive
than robots (with and without salient mechanical body parts) by men if asked explicitly. On the other
hand, the implicit measure did not yield any differences in the associative strength for the concept
of attractiveness between women and robots. This highlights the importance of human-like visual
cues for sex robots, as they have been found to play an important role in (sexual) attractiveness ratings
(breast size, hips) between humans. Moreover, we demonstrated that personal characteristics related to
social life, such as loneliness, do not play a significant role in influencing the ratings of attractiveness of
sex robots. Instead, we found that the negative attitude towards robots is the main user characteristic
that predicts the attractiveness ratings of sex robots. Taken together, the present study contributes to
the almost non-existent empirical research on sexual aspects of human-robot interaction and offers
many further research questions that need to be addressed in the future in order to gain a deeper
understanding of users’ acceptance of sex robots.
Author Contributions: J.S. and N.K. conceived and designed the experiments; J.S. performed the experiments;
J.S. analyzed the data; J.S. wrote the original paper, N.K. reviewed and edited the paper, N.K. supervised.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Abbreviations
The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
MDPI Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute
DOAJ Directory of open access journals
TLA Three-letter acronym
LD linear dichroism
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 16 of 18
References
1. Barss, P. The Erotic Engine: How Pornography has Powered Mass Communication, from Gutenberg to Google;
Doubleday Canada: New York, NY, USA, 2011.
2. Worthen, M.G.F. Sexual Deviance and Society: A Sociological Examination; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2016.
3. Levy, D. Love and Sex with Robots: The Evolution of Human-Robot Relationships; Harper: New York, NY,
USA, 2007.
4. Lonely Men to Get Guide on Building a Sex Robot. The Times. Available online: http://www.thetimes.co.
UK/article/lonely-men-to-get-guide-on-building-a-sex-robot-hn69zggs0 (accessed on 30 September 2016).
5. Schuetz, M.; Arnold, T. Are We Ready for Sex Robots? In Proceedings of the Eleventh ACM/IEEE
International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Christchurch, New Zealand, 7–10 March 2016.
6. Reeves, B.; Nass, C. How People Treat Computers and New Media Like Real People and Places; CSLI Publications
and Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996; pp. 19–36.
7. Mori, M. The Uncanny Valley. Energy 1970, 7, 33–35.
8. Richardson, K. The asymmetrical ‘relationship’: Parallels between prostitution and the development of sex
robots. ACM SIGCAS Comput. Soc. 2016, 45, 290–293. [CrossRef]
9. Sullins, J.P. Applied Professional Ethics for the Reluctant Roboticist. In Proceedings of the Emerging Policy
and Ethics of Human-Robot Interaction, Workshop at HRI, Portland, OR, USA, 2–5 March 2015.
10. Berlant, L.; Warner, M. Sex in public. Crit. Inq. 1998, 24, 547–566. [CrossRef]
11. Schaefer, M.T.; Olson, D.H. Assessing Intimacy: The PAIR Inventory. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 1981, 7, 47–60.
[CrossRef]
12. Yava¸sçao˘ glue, I.; Oktay, B.; Sim¸sek, Ü.; Ozyurt, M. Role of ejaculation in the treatment of chronic non-bacterial
prostatitis. Int. J. Urol. 1999, 6, 130–134. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
13. García, H.; Soriano, E.; Arriaza, G. Friends with Benefits and PsychologicalWellbeing. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci.
2014, 132, 241–247. [CrossRef]
14. DeLamater, J.D.; Sill, M. Sexual desire in later life. J. Sex Res. 2005, 42, 138–149. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
15. Motofei, I.G.; Rowland, D.L. The physiological basis of human sexual arousal: Neuroendocrine sexual
asymmetry. Int. J. Androl. 2005, 28, 78–87. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
16. Kirkendall, L.A. Premarital Intercourse and Interpersonal Relationships: A Research Study of Interpersonal
Relationships Based on Case Histories of 668 Premarital Intercourse Experiences Reported by 200 College Level
Males; Julian Press: New York, NY, USA, 1961.
17. Marelich, W.D.; Lundquist, J. Motivations for Sexual Intimacy: Development of a Needs-Based Sexual
Intimacy Scale. Int. J. Sex. Health 2008, 20, 177–186. [CrossRef]
18. Joseph, S. Social Work Practice and Men Who Have Sex with Men; Sage Publications: New Delhi, India, 2005.
19. Singh, D. Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. J. Personal.
Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 293–307. [CrossRef]
20. Dixson, B.J.; Grimshaw, G.M.; Linklater, W.L.; Dixson, A.F. Eye-tracking of men's preferences for waist-to-hip
ratio and breast size of women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2011, 40, 43–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
21. Hewig, J.; Trippe, R.H.; Hecht, H.; Straube, T.; Miltner, W.H. Gender differences for specific body regions
when looking at men and women. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2008, 32, 67–78. [CrossRef]
22. George, P.A.; Hole, G.J. The influence of feature-based information in the age processing of unfamiliar faces.
Perception 1998, 27, 295–312. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
23. Ekman, P. Facial expression and emotion. Am. Psychol. 1993, 48, 384–392. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
24. Kramer, R.S.; Jones, A.L.; Ward, R. A lack of sexual dimorphism in the width-to-height ratio in white European
faces using 2D photographs, 3D scans, and anthropometry. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e42705. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
25. Berscheid, E.; Reis, H.T. Attraction and close relationships. In The Handbook of Social Psychology; Gilbert, D.T.,
Fiske, S.T., Lindzey, G., Eds.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 193–281.
26. Snowden, R.J.; Wichter, J.; Gray, N.S. Implicit and explicit measurements of sexual preference in gay and
heterosexual men: A comparison of priming techniques and the implicit association task. Arch. Sex. Behav.
2008, 37, 558–565. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
27. Nass, C.; Moon, Y. Machines and mindlessness: Social responses to computers. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 81–103.
[CrossRef]
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 17 of 18
28. Von der Pütten, A.R. Uncannily Human—Experimental Investigation of the Uncanny Valley Phenomenon.
Ph.D. Thesis, University of Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany, 2014.
29. Ham, J.; van Esch, M.; Limpens, Y.; de Pee, J.; Cabibihan, J.J.; Ge, S.S. The Automaticity of Social Behavior
towards Robots: The Influence of Cognitive Load on Interpersonal Distance to Approachable versus Less
Approachable Robots. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Social Robotics, Chengdu, China,
29–31 October 2012.
30. Li, J.; Ju, W.; Reeves, B. Touching a Mechanical Body: Tactile Contact of a Human-Shaped Robot is
Physiologically Arousing. In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the International Communication
Association, Fukuoka, Japan, 9–13 June 2016.
31. Werner, K.; Oberzaucher, J.; Werner, F. Evaluation of human-robot interaction factors of a socially assistive
robot together with older people. In Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Complex, Intelligent
and Software Intensive Systems (CISIS), Palermo, Italy, 4–6 July 2012.
32. Ferrari, F. Too Human to be a Machine? Social Robots, Anthropomorphic Appearance, and Concerns on
the Negative Impact of This Technology on Humans and Their Identity. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Trento,
Trento, Italy, 2015.
33. Bartneck, C.; Bleeker, T.; Bun, J.; Fens, P.; Riet, L. The influence of robot anthropomorphism on the feelings of
embarrassment when interacting with robots. Paladyn J. Behav. Robot. 2010, 1, 109–115. [CrossRef]
34. Bartneck, C.; Kanda, T.; Ishiguro, H.; Hagita, N. Is The Uncanny Valley An Uncanny Cliff? In Proceedings
of the 16th IEEE International Conference on Robot & Human Interactive Communication, Jeju, Korea,
26–29 August 2007.
35. Minsky, M. A Framework for Representing Knowledge. In Psychology of Computer Vision; McGraw-Hill:
New York, NY, USA, 1975; pp. 211–281.
36. Bartneck, C.; Kanda, T.; Ishiguro, H.; Hagita, N. My Robotic Doppelgänger—A Critical Look at the
Uncanny Valley. In Proceedings of the 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive
Communication, RO-MAN2009, Toyama, Japan, 27 September–2 October; pp. 269–276.
37. Ferguson, A. The Sex Doll: A History; McFarland & Co.: Jefferson, NC, USA, 2010.
38. Bardzell, S.; Bardzell, J. Technosexuality. In The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Gender and Sexuality Studies;
Wong, A., Wickramasinghe, M., Hoogland, R., Naples, N.A., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Singapore, 2016;
pp. 1–3.
39. Reece, M.; Herbenick, D.; Dodge, B.; Sanders, S.A.; Ghassemi, A.; Fortenberry, J.D. Vibrator use among
heterosexual men varies by partnership status: Results from a nationally representative study in the United
States. J. Sex Marital Ther. 2012, 36, 389–407. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
40. Gillespie, C. Lars and the Real Girl. Available online: http://www.imdb.com/title/tt0805564/ (accessed on
8 February 2017).
41. Holt, N. Love Me, Love My Doll/ Guys and Dolls. Available online: http://www.imdb.com/title/tt0968743/
(accessed on 8 February 2017).
42. Eyssel, F.; Reich, N. Loneliness makes the heart grow fonder (or robots): On the effects of loneliness on
psychological anthropomorphism. In Proceedings of the 8th ACM/IEEE International Conference on
Human-Robot Interaction, Tokyo, Japan, 3–6 March 2013.
43. Baumeister, R.F.; Leary, M.R. The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental
human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 117, 497–529. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
44. Kolbeck, S. Zur Psychometrischen Differenzierbarkeit von Sozialen Ängsten und Sozialen Defiziten. [About
Psychometrical Differentiability of Social Fears and Social Deficits]. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Hamburg,
Hamburg, Germany, 2008.
45. Leary, M.R.; Dobbins, S.E. Social anxiety, sexual behavior, and contraceptive use. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
1983, 45, 1347–1354. [CrossRef]
46. Suzuki, T.; Yamada, S.; Kanda, T.; Nomura, T. Influence of Social Avoidance and Distress on People’s
Preferences for Robots as Daily Life Communication Partners. In Proceedings of the New Friends 2015—The
First International Conference on Social Robots in Therapy and Education, Almere, The Netherlands,
22–23 October 2015.
47. Syrdal, D.S.; Dautenhahn, K.; Koay, K.L.; Walters, M.L. The negative attitudes towards robots scale and
reactions to robot behavior in a live human-robot interaction study. In Proceedings of the New Frontiers in
Human-Robot Interaction, a Symposium at the AISB 2009 Convention, Edinburgh, UK, 6–9 April 2009.
Multimodal Technologies and Interact. 2017, 1, 3 18 of 18
48. Nomura, T.; Suzuki, T.; Kanda, T.; Kato, K. Measurement of negative attitudes toward robots. Interact. Stud.
2006, 7, 437–454. [CrossRef]
49. Nomura, T.; Shintani, T.; Fujii, K.; Hokabe, K. Experimental investigation of relationships between anxiety,
negative attitudes, and allowable distance of robots. In Proceedings of the 2nd IASTED International
Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, Chamonix, France, 14–16 March 2007.
50. Horn, J.L. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 1965, 30, 179–185.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
51. Neave, N.; Jackson, R.; Saxton, T.; Hönekopp, J. The influence of anthropomorphic tendencies on human
hoarding behaviors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 72, 214–219. [CrossRef]
52. Russel, D.; Peplau, L.A.; Cutrona, C.E. The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant
validity evidence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 472–480. [CrossRef]
53. Krämer, N.C.; Hoffmann, L.; Fuchslocher, A.; Eimler, S.C.; Szczuka, J.M.; Brand, M. Do I need to belong?
Development of a Scale for Measuring the Need to Belong and its Predictive Value for Media Usage.
In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the International Communication Association, London, UK,
17–21 June 2013.
54. Fazio, R.H.; Sanbonmatsu, D.M.; Powell, M.C.; Kardes, F.R. On the automatic activation of attitudes.
J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 50, 229–238. [CrossRef]
55. Avero, P.; Calvo, M.G. Affective Priming with Pictures of Emotional Scenes: The Role of Perceptual Similarity
and Category Relatedness. Span. J. Psychol. 2006, 9, 10. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
© 2017 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open-access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Note: A sex doll is a type of sex toy in the size and shape of a sexual partner for aid in masturbation.